The Iowa Gambling Task:
A Critical Revision
Keywords:
Iowa Gambling Task, Decision-making, Feedback, Rewards, PunishmentsAbstract
The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) is a widely used task in the assessment of the decision-making ability. In this article, we conduct a literature review by comparing IGT versions, different performance assessment measures, and changes to procedures including feedback, spatial randomization of decks, number of trials, number of cards per deck, instructions, payment, and rewards and punishments. On basis of this analysis, we conclude that different versions of the task, changes in application procedures and different measures used to assess the task have an impact on performance, thereby affecting comparison among studies and generalization of results. Finally, we offer suggestions to define adequate procedures.
Downloads
References
Bakos, D. S., Denburg, N., Fonseca, R. P., & Parente, M. A. d. M. P. (2010). A cultural study on decision making: performance differences on the Iowa Gambling Task between selected groups of Brazilians and Americans. Psychology & Neuroscience, 3(1), 101-107. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2010.1.013
Bakos, D. S., Parente, M. A. d. M. P., & Bertagnolli, A. C. (2010). A tomada de decisão em adultos jovens e em adultos idosos: Um estudo comparativo. Psicologia, Ciência e Profissão, 30(1), 162-173. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_serial&lng=pt&pid=1414-9893
Balodis, I. M., MacDonald, T. K., & Olmstead, M. C. (2006). Instructional cues modify performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 60(2), 109-117. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.007
Bark, R., Dieckmann, S., Bogerts, B., & Northoff, G. (2005). Deficit in decision making in catatonic schizophrenia: An exploratory study. Psychiatry Research, 134(2), 131-141. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.04.013
Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 30-40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001
Bechara, A. (2007). Iowa Gambling Task. Professional Manual. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Bechara, A., & Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part I): Impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1675-1689. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00015-5
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7-15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(13), 5473-5481. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 428-437. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275(5304), 1293-1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: Some questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 159-162; discussion 162-154. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.002
Bechara, A., Dolan, S., & Hindes, A. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part II): Myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1690-1705. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00016-7
Bechara, A., Dolan, S., Denburg, N., Hindes, A., Anderson, S. W., & Nathan, P. E. (2001). Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia, 39(4), 376-389. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00136-6
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123(11), 2189-2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189
Blair, R., Colledge, E., & Mitchell, D. (2001). Somatic markers and response reversal: Is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 499-511. doi: 10.1023/A:1012277125119
Bolla, K. I., Eldreth, D. A., London, E. D., Kiehl, K. A., Mouratidis, M., Contoreggi, C., . . . Ernst, M. (2003). Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task. Neuroimage, 19(3), 1085-1094. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00113-7
Bos, R. van den, Houx, B. B., & Spruijt, B. M. (2006). The effect of reward magnitude differences on choosing disadvantageous decks in the Iowa Gambling Task. Biological Psychology, 71(2), 155-161. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.05.003
Bowman, C. H., & Turnbull, O. H. (2003). Real versus facsimile reinforcers on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 207-210. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00111-8
Brand, M., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2006). Neuropsychological correlates of decision-making in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Networks, 19(8), 1266-1276. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.001
Campbell, M. C., Stout, J. C., & Finn, P. R. (2004). Reduced autonomic responsiveness to Gambling Task losses in Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 239-245. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102105
Caroselli, J. S., Hiscock, M., Scheibel, R. S., & Ingram, F. (2006). The Simulated Gambling Paradigm applied to young adults: An examination of University students’ performance. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(4), 203-212. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1304_1
Carter, S., & Pasqualini, M. S. (2004). Stronger autonomic response accompanies better learning: A test of Damasio’s somatic marker hypothesis. Cognition & Emotion, 18(7), 901-911. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000338
Carvalho, J. C. N. (2010). Tomada de decisão no Iowa Gambling Task: Estudos comparativos quanto à s varáveis idade e escolaridade e estudo correlacional. Dissertação de Mestrado, Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brasil. Retrieved from http://tede.pucrs.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=2765
Cella, M., Dymond, S., Cooper, A., & Turnbull, O. (2007). Effects of decision-phase time constraints on emotion-based learning in the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 64(2), 164-169. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.003
Chiu, Y. C., & Lin, C. H. (2007). Is deck C an advantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task? Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(37), 1-11. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-37
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1-73. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00664-8
Crone, E. A., & van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: Performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(3), 251-279. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2
Crone, E. A., Bunge, S. A., Latenstein, H., & van der Molen, M. W. (2005). Characterization of children’s decision making: Sensitivity to punishment frequency, not task complexity. Child Neuropsychology, 11(3), 245-263. doi: 10.1080/092970490911261
Crone, E. A., Vendel, I., & van der Molen, M. W. (2003). Decision-making in disinhibited adolescents and adults: Insensitivity to future consequences or driven by immediate reward? Personality and Individual Differences, 35(7), 1625-1641. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00386-0
Dalgleish, T., Yiend, J., Bramham, J., Teasdale, J. D., Ogilvie, A. D., Malhi, G., & Howard, R. (2004). Neuropsychological processing associated with recovery from depression after stereotactic subcaudate tractotomy. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(10), 1913-1916. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/
Damásio, A. (2003). O erro de Descartes. Emoção, razão e cérebro humano (D. Vicente & G. Segurado, Trad.). Mem Martins, Portugal: Publicações Europa América (Trabalho original publicado em 1994)
Davis, C., Patte, K., Tweed, S., & Curtis, C. (2007). Personality traits associated with decision-making deficits. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(2), 279-290. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.006
Denburg, N. L., Recknor, E. C., Bechara, A., & Tranel, D. (2006). Psychophysiological anticipation of positive outcomes promotes advantageous decision-making in normal older persons. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(1), 19-25. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.021
Denburg, N. L., Tranel, D., & Bechara, A. (2005). The ability to decide advantageously declines prematurely in some normal older persons. Neuropsychologia, 43(7), 1099-1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.012
Dretsch, M. N., & Tipples, J. (2008). Working memory involved in predicting future outcomes based on past experiences. Brain and Cognition, 66(1), 83-90. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.05.006
Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(2), 239-271. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001
Evans, C. E. Y., Kemish, K., & Turnbull, O. H. (2004). Paradoxical effects of education on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 54, 240-244. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.022
Fernie, G., & Tunney, R. J. (2006). Some decks are better than others: The effect of reinforcer type and task instructions on learning in the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 60(1), 94-102. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.09.011
Garon, N., & Moore, C. (2004). Complex decision-making in early childhood. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 158-170. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00272-0
Geurts, H. M., Oord, S. van der, & Crone, E. A. (2006). Hot and cool aspects of cognitive control in children with ADHD: Decision-making and inhibition. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(6), 813-824. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9059-2
Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102(4), 684-704. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.684
Glicksohn, J., & Zilberman, N. (2010). Gambling on individual differences in decision making. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(5), 557-562. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.006
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108(3), 356-388. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.108.3.356
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1984). Automatic processing of fundamental information ”“ The case of frequency of occurrence. American Psychological Association, 39(12), 1372-1388. Retirado de http://www.psych.utoronto.ca/users/hasherlab/
Hooper, C. J., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., & Yarger, R. S. (2004). Adolescents’ performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: Implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1148-1158. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148
Huizenga, H. M., Crone, E. A., & Jansen, B. J. (2007). Decision-making in healthy children, adolescents and adults explained by the use of increasingly complex proportional reasoning rules. Developmental Science, 10(6), 814-825. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00621.x
Jollant, F., Bellivier, F., Leboyer, M., Astruc, B., Torres, S., Verdier, R., . . . Courtet, P. (2005). Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2), 304-310. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. Retrieved from http://www.pattonfunds.com/EDGE/EDGE_pdfs/ProspectTheory.pdf
Lin, C. H., Chiu, Y. C., Lee, P. L., & Hsieh, J. C. (2007). Is deck B a disadvantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task? Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(16), 1-10. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-16
Maia, T. V., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: What participants really know in the Iowa Gambling Task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101(45), 16075-16080. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406666101
Mazas, C. A., Finn, P. R., & Steinmetz, J. E. (2000). Decision-making biases, antisocial personality, and early-onset alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(7), 1036-1040. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb04647.x
Mitchell, D. G. V., Colledge, E., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2002). Risky decisions and response reversal: Is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2013-2022. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00056-8
Monterosso, J., Ehrman, R., Napier, K. L., O’Brien, C. P., & Childress, A. R. (2001). Three decision-making tasks in cocaine-dependent patients: Do they measure the same construct? Addiction, 96(12), 1825-1837. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9612182512.x
Noel, X., Bechara, A., Dan, B., Hanak, C., & Verbanck, P. (2007). Response inhibition deficit is involved in poor decision making under risk in nonamnesic individuals with alcoholism. Neuropsychology, 21(6), 778-786. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.778
O’Carroll, R. E., & Papps, B. P. (2003). Decision making in humans: The effect of manipulating the central noradrenergic system. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 74, 376-378. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.3.376
Oldershaw, A., Grima, E., Jollant, F., Richards, C., Simic, M., Taylor, L., & Schmidt, U. (2009). Decision making and problem solving in adolescents who deliberately self-harm. Psychological Medicine, 39(1), 95-104. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003693
Overman, W. H. (2004). Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 134-147. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00279-3
Overman, W. H., Frassrand, K., Ansel, S., Trawalter, S., Bies, B., & Redmond, A. (2004). Performance on the Iowa Card Task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia, 42(13), 1838-1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.014
Overman, W. H., Graham, L., Redmond, A., Eubank, R., Boettcher, L., Samplawski, O., & Walsh, K. (2006). Contemplation of moral dilemmas eliminates sex differences on the Iowa Gambling Task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(4), 817-825. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044 .120.4.817
Pecchinenda, A., Dretsch, M., & Chapman, P. (2006). Working memory involvement in emotion-based processes underlying choosing advantageously. Experimental Psychology, 53(3), 191-197. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.3.191
Petry, N. M., Bickel, W. K., & Arnett, M. (1998). Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. Addiction, 93(5), 729-738. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.9357298.x
Ritter, L. M., Meador-Woodruff, J. H., & Dalack, G. W. (2004). Neurocognitive measures of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 68(1), 65-73. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00086-0
Schmitt, W. A., Brinkley, C. A., & Newman, J. P. (1999). Testing Damasio’s somatic marker hypothesis with psychopathic individuals: Risk takers or risk averse? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(3), 538-543. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/abn/index.aspx
Schneider, D. D. G., & Parente, M. A. d. M. P. (2006). O desempenho de adultos jovens e idosos na Iowa Gambling Task (IGT): Um estudo sobre a tomada de decisão. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 19, 442-450. doi: 10.1590/S0102-79722006000300013
Schneider, J. W. (1973). Reinforcer effectiveness as a function of reinforcer rate and magnitude: A comparison of concurrent performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20(3), 461-471. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1973.20-461
Singh, V., & Khan, A. (2009). Heterogeneity in choices on Iowa Gambling Task: Preference for infrequent-high magnitude punishment. Mind & Society, 8(1), 43-57. doi: 10.1007/s11299-008-0050-1
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 311-322. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x
Steinhauer, G. D. (1984). Preference for predictacle small rewards over unpredictable larger rewards. Psychological Reports, 54(2), 467-471. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1984.54.2.467
Stocco, A., Fum, D., & Napoli, A. (2009). Dissociable processes underlying decisions in the Iowa Gambling Task: A new integrative framework. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5(1), 1-12. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-1
Stout, J. C., Rodawalt, W. C., & Siemers, E. R. (2001). Risky decision making in Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(01), 92-101. doi: 10.1017/S1355617701711095
Suzuki, A., Hirota, A., Takasawa, N., & Shigemasu, K. (2003). Application of the somatic marker hypothesis to individual differences in decision making. Biological Psychology, 65(1), 81-88. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00093-0
Toplak, M. E., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2005). Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with Attention-Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behavioral and Brain Function, 1(8), 1-12. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-1-8
Vadhan, N. P., Hart, C. L., Haney, M., van Gorp, W. G., & Foltin, R. W. (2009). Decision-making in long-term cocaine users: Effects of a cash monetary contingency on Gambling Task performance. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 102(1-3), 95-101. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.02.003
Verdejo-Garcia, A., Aguillar de Arcos, F., & Pérez-Garcia, M. (2004). Alteraciones de los procesos de toma de decisiones vinculados al córtex prefrontal ventromedial en pacientes drogodependientes. Revista de Neurologia, 38(7), 601-606. Retrieved from http://www.revneurol.com/
Verdejo-Garcia, A., Benbrook, A., Funderburk, F., David, P., Cadet, J. L., & Bolla, K. I. (2007). The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 90(1), 2-11. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.02.004
Wagar, B. M., & Dixon, M. (2006). Affective guidance in the Iowa Gambling Task. Cognitive, Affective, Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(4), 277-290. Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com/content/1530-7026/
Wagner, G. P., & Parente, M. A. d. M. P. (2009). O desempenho de idosos quanto a tomada de decisão em duas variáveis do Iowa Gambling Test. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 25(3), 425-433. doi: 10.1590/S0102-37722009000300017
Weller, J. A., Levin, I. P., & Bechara, A. (2010). Do individual differences in Iowa Gambling Task performance predict adaptive decision making for risky gains and losses? Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(2), 141-150. doi: 10.1080/13803390902881926
Wilder, K. E., Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (1998). Operant conditioning and the orbitofrontal cortex in schizophrenic patients: Unexpected evidence for intact functioning. Schizophrenia Research, 30(2), 169-174. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(97)00135-7
Wood, S., Busemeyer, J., Koling, A., Cox, C., & Davis, H. (2005). Older adults as adaptive decision-makers: Evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Psychology and Aging, 20(2), 220-225. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.220
Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Sanft, H. (1982). Automatic encoding of event frequency: Further findings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(2), 106-116. Retrieved from http://www.psych.utoronto.ca/users/hasherlab/publications.htm