Article
Retrieval practice at school: a narrative review of
test formats
Prática de
lembrar em ambiente escolar: revisão narrativa sobre formatos de testes
Práctica de recuperación en escuelas: revisión
narrativa sobre formatos de las pruebas
Anne Caroline Borba da Silva[i]
Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.
Jacarezinho, PR, Brazil
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0689-1196
Lisandra Tamires Mendonça[ii]
Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.
Jacarezinho, PR, Brazil
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7880-5955
José Cláudio de Sene Miguel[iii]
Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.
Jacarezinho, PR, Brazil
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6097-2950
Roberta Ekuni[iv]
Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.
Jacarezinho, PR, Brazil
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1490-0184
The authors contributed
equally to the elaboration of the manuscript.
Received:
04/21/2022
Accepted:
07/28/2022
Published: 08/16/2022
Linhas Críticas | Journal edited by the
Faculty of Education of the University of Brasília, Brazil
ISSN: 1516-4896 | e-ISSN: 1981-0431
Volume 28, 2022 (jan-dec).
http://periodicos.unb.br/index.php/linhascriticas
Full reference (APA):
Silva, A. C. B. da, Mendonça, L. T., Miguel, J. C. de S., & Ekuni,
R. (2022). Retrieval practice at school: a narrative review of test formats. Linhas Críticas, 28, e43014. https://doi.org/10.26512/lc28202243014
Alternative link:
https://periodicos.unb.br/index.php/linhascriticas/article/view/43014
Creative Commons license
CC BY 4.0.
Abstract: Retrieval practice, which consists of trying to remember
content already seen, is considered as one of the most effective strategies to
generate long-term learning. In order to promote its effective use, this
narrative review focuses on suggesting guidelines for the application of
different test formats, often used to practice retrieval, and pointing out
advantages and disadvantages of each. As a result, the reviewed research
encompassed students from kindergarten through undergraduation. Overall,
retrieval practice can benefit the learning of different content, providing
test formats are suitable to the learner's age, regardless of the materials
used.
Keywords: Education.
Test format. Learning.
Resumo: A prática de lembrar, que consiste em tentar
recordar conteúdo já visto, é tida como uma das estratégias mais eficazes para
gerar aprendizado de longa duração. A fim de promover seu uso eficaz, a
presente revisão narrativa tem por foco sugerir diretrizes a respeito da
aplicação de diferentes formatos de testes, com frequência empregados para
praticar lembrar, e apontar vantagens e desvantagens de cada um. Como
resultado, os estudos revisados envolveram alunos desde a educação infantil até
a graduação. No geral, a prática de lembrar pode beneficiar a aprendizagem de
diferentes conteúdos contanto que os formatos de teste se adaptem à idade do
aprendiz, independentemente dos materiais usados.
Palavras-chave: Educação. Formato de teste. Aprendizagem.
Resumen: La práctica de recuperación, es decir, intentar
recordar contenido ya visto, es considerada una de las estrategias más
efectivas para generar aprendizaje de larga duración. Para promover su uso
eficaz, esta revisión narrativa se enfoca en sugerir pautas sobre la aplicación
de diferentes formatos de pruebas, a menudo usados para practicar recordar, y
señalar sus ventajas y desventajas. Los estudios revisados involucraron a
alumnos desde el jardín de infantes hasta la graduación. En general, la
práctica de recuperación puede beneficiar el aprendizaje de diferentes
contenidos siempre que los formatos de prueba se adapten a la edad del alumno,
independientemente de los materiales utilizados.
Palabras
clave: Educación. Formato de prueba. Aprendizaje.
Introduction
One of the goals of education is for learning to
persist over time. However, it is common for students to complain about not
being able to remember the content they have studied. One of the reasons is
that they do not prefer to use the best strategies to learn (Karpicke et al.,
2009; Dunlosky et al., 2013; Karpicke et
al., 2014; Ekuni et al., 2020). Given that, several research in the field of
Cognitive Psychology, both in laboratories and educational settings, point to
strategies that promote long-lasting learning, such as retrieval practice
(Dunlosky et al., 2013).
Retrieval practice is a learning
strategy that aims to try to remember content previously seen, either through
tests (multiple-choice, fill-in-the-blanks, short-answer, etc.) or through
exercises that stimulate retrieval (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a). During the
encoding of information (which occurs, for example, when attending a class,
reading a text, etc.), information is put in our head. When trying to remember,
however, we search for this information, trying to put it out of our
head (Agarwal & Bain, 2019). In this attempt, our brain elaborates and
relates available routes (pathways) to identify information we have previously
accessed (Bjork, 1975) and activates semantically related content (Carpenter,
2011). This means that when we find the content we are looking for in our mind,
information is reconsolidated, its access is facilitated, and the memory trace
is strengthened, making it more lasting (Van den Broek et al., 2016).
Historically, experiments testing the effects of
retrieval practice date back more than a century. The papers reviewed converge
on an experiment conducted by Abbott (1909), which is indicated as the initial
milestone of studies in this field (Yang et al., 2021). Abbott's finding, later
replicated by numerous researchers, is that testing learned knowledge alters
its retention in memory (Abbott, 1909; Roediger & Karpicke, 2011). The
heyday of research on remembering practice, however, would only come up in the
1960s and 1970s from the publication of other relevant papers regarding topics
pertinent to the Cognitive Psychology of Memory (Roediger & Karpicke,
2011). Despite a century of research in the field, Agarwal and Bain (2019) state
that pedagogical “fads" are shown in teacher education courses and end up
leaving evidence-based teaching strategies out of it. Perhaps this explains why
retrieval practice is not widely seen as a teaching strategy in everyday school
life.
Among its benefits, retrieval practice improves
metacognition (McDermott, 2021), decreases anxiety on later tests (Agarwal et
al., 2014), provides feedback on what one knows and what one does not
know, thereby making room for future studies (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006b). Another advantage is that it involves no
additional financial investment (Roediger & Pyc, 2012) and can be adapted
to a variety of educational materials and teaching methods (Agarwal et al.,
2018).
As stated, one of the ways to practice retrieve can
be through tests, for when a student sees a certain question, he must try to
extract the answer from his head. However, in educational practice, tests are
usually used as a form of assessment (i.e., school exams), not as a learning
strategy (Yang et al., 2021). Implementing retrieval practice in education is
important in the sense of promoting long- lasting learning and encouraging
students to engage in their own cognitive processes through effective study
strategies (Ekuni et al., 2020).
Education needs scientifically proven methods that
enable concrete effects in the school environment (Slavin, 2020). Therefore,
the use of more effective learning strategies can benefit students. In view of
the above, the present study aimed to conduct a narrative review, a style of
review that does not require following a systematic protocol, so that it is not
necessary to inform the search methodology of the references (Rother, 2007).
This review can be done by the author himself, without having to explain the
criteria for the search and selection of sources (Collins & Fauser, 2005).
However, one should ensure academic eloquence from the author's critical
analysis of the published literature (Rother, 2007). Likewise, care must be
taken so that the sources selected are based on scientific studies that have
proven to be effective (Slavin, 2020). Following these guidelines, it is
possible to establish a relationship between productions on previously defined
topics, consolidate concepts and provide practical guidance (Elias et al., 2012).
Thus, in narrative reviews, the bibliographic productions are analyzed in such
a way that they result in a state-of-the-art on the topic in which the
researcher intends to delve (Elias et al., 2012).
To analyze the use of different test formats as learning
strategies, as explained above, we used the narrative review method. As
inclusion criteria we used papers that focused on tests as a learning strategy,
and not as an assessment tool. In fact, the purpose of using these strategies
is not to evaluate the student, but to encourage him to try to remember the
content learned. In this sense, the literature knows this phenomenon as testing-effect, test-enhanced learning,
retrieval-based learning, retrieval
practice, terms we used in the searches we did in databases through a
non-systematic way. Finally, we provide recommendations and tips to teachers
and students to implement retrieval practice in the teaching and learning
process.
True or false
True or false (T/F) questions
are answer selection questions, meaning that the respondent must recognize the
answer to be selected by deciding whether a statement is false or true
(Santrock, 2009). For example: Castle Geyser is the oldest geyser in
Yellowstone Park: (T) / (F).
From the point of
view of their advantages, questions of this type are commonly used in the
classroom and are pedagogically practical to administer because they are
objective in measuring results (score is based on right answers) and require
less prep time (Uner et al., 2022). Furthermore, this type of test allows a
large number of questions to be covered in a common testing period (Santrock,
2009). If there are situations where it is difficult to create several lures,
i.e., plausible alternatives to multiple choice questions, this type of test is
also useful (Uner et al., 2022).
T/F tests result in
benefits also for children. For example, research involving students (eleven years-old on average) in a classroom setting found
positive effects on cognitive (correctness of answers) and metacognitive (level
of confidence in answers) performance from experiments involving T/F questions
on History, Politics, and Geography content (Barenberg & Dutke, 2019). As
indicated by experiments conducted with nearly five hundred university participants employing
Biology content in virtual learning environments, T/F questions can also
be beneficial in e-learning or b-learning scenarios, especially when
feedback is present (Enders et al., 2021).
Conversely, the
pedagogical effectiveness of T/F tests may not be as powerful compared to other
test formats, as indicated by experiments conducted with undergraduates in a
laboratory setting using texts (Brabec et al., 2021). Thus, among the possible disadvantages of applying this type of test are blind
guesses (i.e., attempts to get it right at random) and negative suggestions. In
the latter case, by reading the lure (false information) the student may learn
it and recognize it in other tests in the future,
taking it for granted (Uner et al., 2022). In this vein, in experiments
conducted with undergraduates, Brabec et al. (2021) found evidence of negative
suggestion when participants had to choose the true alternative compared to not
being tested with T/F questions (control condition). According to the authors,
this negative effect may be lessened with the presence of feedback (Brabec
et al., 2021).
To try to minimize this
catch, as well as to avoid blind guesses, an experiment conducted with
Psychology students, demanded participants to provide a justification for each
selected answer (Schaap et al., 2014). As can be seen, this is a slightly
modified T/F test. However, this strategy did not generate significant benefit
compared to participants who had not had to justify their choices (Schaap et
al., 2014). In attention to this, other modified experiments have required
undergraduates not to justify choices, but to correct false alternatives (Uner
et al., 2022).
Other modifications
experimented involved inserting competitive clauses into the statements, a
strategy that may make the tested information last longer in memory (Brabec et
al., 2021). For example, instead of the statement standing alone, there is
another statement inserted into it, in parentheses: Castle geyser (not Stemboat)
is the tallest geyser (example taken from the aforementioned paper).
It is relevant to
note that when students are prompted to correct alternatives which they think
are false, the presentation of feedback improves retention of correct
items on a subsequent short-answer test compared to a regular T/F procedure
(Uner et al., 2022). There are three types of feedback: the first, the right/wrong
feedback type; the second one, called corrective feedback, which
provides the student the correct answer; and the third one, called elaborate
feedback, which explains why a certain answer is true or false (Enders et
al., 2021).
Corrective feedback
can improve retention of tested items in T/F questions. Likewise, simple
modifications (such as inserting competitive clauses and requesting correction
of the wrong alternative or justification of the choice) in the way the test is
administered appear to be more effective and promising for classroom
implementation (Uner et al., 2022).
Regarding to
suggestions for writing T/F questions, here is what we
recommend to teachers: keep only one main idea in each alternative, and do not insert several of
them at the same time; make statements short and with understandable
vocabulary; avoid absolute terms (always, never, no one, etc.),
circumstantial modal words (can, maybe, sometimes, etc.), and double negatives (Santrock, 2009).
Multiple-choice
Multiple-choice (MC) questions
also involve answer selection. In terms of form, this type of question is
composed of two parts: the base (statement) and its set of possible answers, of
which only one is correct, and the others are lures (Santrock, 2009). MC
tests are often used by educators because, like T/F, they allow easier scoring
and are perceived as more objective (Butler et al., 2006). In fact, it is the
most used type of objective test (Marsh et al., 2007). In addition, MC quizzes
can be used throughout the school year to benefit student learning and
performance on final tests, whether they are MC (composed of recognition
questions) or short-answer (composed of elaboration questions) (McDermott et
al., 2014).
MC questions should be
worded so that lures are plausible (Little et al., 2012).
However, as already mentioned in the excerpt about T/F type questions, there is
the inconvenient effect of negative suggestion. Lures (incorrect answers) can be
learned and reproduced on a later test, so that the MC test can be
counterproductive if there are too many of them, as their interference can lead
to the assimilation of erroneous knowledge (e.g., Marsh et al., 2012a). Indeed,
an experiment conducted with undergraduates points out that
the greater the number of alternatives offered in the MC test, the less
beneficial the test is (Roediger & Marsh, 2005). In addition to the
interference caused by too many lures, insufficient study of the content prior
to the test contributes to the learning of misinformation (Butler &
Roediger, 2008).
To avoid negative
suggestion and leverage the benefits of MC tests, it is important to provide
feedback (Marsh et al., 2012a). Feedback, particularly corrective
feedback (in which the correct answer is given), is useful for correcting
misinformation that students have learned from lures, as well as for
maintaining long-term retention of correct answers (Butler, 2018).
MC tests were employed in
various research studies with different age, setting, and materials
employed. Experiments conducted with children (eight
years-old on average) show that they benefit from MC tests on general
knowledge, coupled with feedback (Marsh et al., 2012b). Other
experiments with teenagers (thirteen years-old on average) point out that
classroom science quizzes accompanied by feedback, even if they are brief,
increase students' performance on later summative assessments, whether MC or
short-answer (McDermott et al., 2014).
As for suggestions on how
to design MC questions, some general and some specific to this type of test can
be listed (Santrock, 2009; Butler, 2018). In general, MC questions should not
contain grammatical improprieties, should be understandable, are best written
as interrogative sentences, and should not contain tricky alternatives.
Specifically, they should have answers of similar length and alternate the
position of the correct alternatives between questions. It should be noted
again that the greater the number of alternatives, the lower the hit rate tends
to be (Butler et al., 2006). Moreover, MC tests should be appropriately
challenging, because if too easy or too difficult, they are useless for both
assessment and promotion of learning (Butler, 2018). A pertinent suggestion is to
affix the "I don't know" option, in order to avoid blind guessing
(Marsh et al., 2007). Again, the purpose of applying tests is learning, not
grading.
As stated earlier, among
its main disadvantages are the elaboration of
the alternatives (both in terms of number, format, and degree of difficulty)
and the possibility of negative suggestion (Butler, 2018). On the other hand,
this test format has the advantage of being easily verifiable. Thus, it
decreases the correction time and increases its objectivity (Butler et al.,
2006; Marsh et al., 2012a). Indeed, it can be applied throughout the school
year to boost learning (McDermott et al., 2014). Finally, marginal knowledge —
that is, the knowledge which, even though stored in memory, is not accessible
at any given time — can be easily reactivated through MC testing (Cantor et
al., 2015).
MC questions have,
therefore, important pedagogical implications, either in quizzes or in final
tests, as they are practical and economical, but require care in their preparation.
The recommendation to teachers, therefore, is to be
careful with the number of alternatives and try to elaborate clear questions
and plausible alternatives, with an adequate level of difficulty for the
students.
Short-answer
The
short-answer (SA) test format is widely used in the classroom. As can be
inferred from its name, it is the same as to answering questions with a short
answer. It is a test that imposes on the student to produce an answer (Larsen
et al., 2008). This type of test is similar to tests with cues. In this case,
however, the question is a cue that directs the respondent to the content to
remember (Moreira et al., 2019). For example, after studying key-term
definitions, 5th graders were asked to type in answers [e.g., What is sound?
____ (form of energy that you can hear and that travels through matter as
waves)] (Lipko-Speeda et al., 2014).
This test
format, like the free-recall test, requires more effort to remember and is more
difficult than the multiple-choice test format, which involves recognition
(Rowland, 2014). Effectively, research points out that SA tests are more
efficient at retaining content than multiple-choice tests (e.g., Kang et al.,
2007; Stenlund et al., 2016). However, there are studies that differ on this
point (see Little et al., 2012). Regarding this disagreement, it is important
to consider the age range of the students and the way the tests are formulated.
Tests should be designed so that students try to remember rather than just
identify answers (Little et al., 2012).
Several
studies with different educational levels have been conducted with the SA test
format, from elementary school (e.g., Goossens et al., 2016), through high
school (Dirkx et al., 2014), to undergraduation (e.g.,
Endres et al., 2020). These researches used as materials: book chapters (e.g.,
Carpenter et al., 2009), word lists (Goossens et al., 2016), key-concept
definition pairs (e.g., Lipko-Speeda et al, 2014),
expository texts (e.g., Dirkx et al., 2014), key-concepts of studied topics
(Wiklund-Hörnqvist et al., 2014), expository lectures (Foss & Pirozzolo,
2017), and lectures (Lyle & Crawford, 2011). Apart from the study by
Goossens et al. (2016), in all of the aforementioned studies there were
benefits of the short-answer test for learning.
Lipko-Speeda
et al. (2014) analyzed the effect of performing SA test with and without
feedback, employing rereading as a control condition. The target audience was
5th grade children, and the questions consisted of definitions of key-concepts
from Science and Geography content. Positive effects were seen only in the test
condition with feedback. In their study conducted with children, Goossens et
al. (2016) found that this test format without feedback was no better than copying the studied material.
Similarly, research conducted with high school students revealed that the SA
test with feedback, compared to rereading and administered after
studying a text on probability, increased retention of content from the text
and improved application of the principles covered in the text (Dirkx et al.,
2014).
Research
with undergraduates has pointed out that, compared to rereading, SA tests were
beneficial for long-term learning and retention (Larsen et al., 2009; Wiklund-Hörnqvist et al., 2014; Greving & Richter, 2018). A
study by Carpenter et al. (2016) showed that SA tests generated better results
with respect to recall of term definitions made by high-performing students.
However, for medium and low-performing students, copying term definitions was
better. In all these studies, feedback was offered after the initial SA
test, except for Greving and Richter's (2018) study. The latter evidenced that,
even without feedback, the short-answer test generated positive effects on
introductory Cognitive Psychology content.
SA tests
with feedback generate more learning outcomes (Kang et al., 2007). In this
sense, because this format involves more retrieval effort, one of the most
important precautions by teachers is to provide feedback with the correct
answers. Therefore, students can correct their mistakes instead of just knowing
what is right or wrong.
The
research cited earlier showed that CR tests with older students were more
efficient (e.g., Greving & Richter, 2018) than with younger students (e.g., Lipko-Speeda et al., 2014). It is therefore
recommended that to younger learners, cues are provided (see section below –
cued-recall) to facilitate recall. Another way out is to provide students with
more learning opportunities and tests until they can integrate and retain the
content (Lipko-Speeda et al., 2014).
One of the
benefits of SA questions is that they favor retention of specific points of a
content previously studied. Consequently, they facilitate the recall of more
difficult or inaccessible points. As an indirect effect, they allow
metacognitive accuracy, that is, the regulation of students' confidence in the
certainty of their answers and the proportion of answers given that they would
remember in a week. In this way, students' judgments regarding their learning
are more calibrated (accurate) in the SA format compared to the free-recall
test format (Endres et al., 2020). Another advantage is that conducting review
at the end of each class via SA questions encourages students to engage in
study and increases their performance on later tests (Greving & Richter,
2018).
One of the
disadvantages of SA quizzes are that, in practice, this format is not very
attractive to students. In addition, it can take teachers up to twice as long
to correct and apply it in class because the format requires more complex
answers (McDermott et al., 2014). However, it is not necessary to correct
individually, as the feedback can be collective (Butler & Roediger, 2007),
since the goal is learning, not grading based on student’s performance. Another
disadvantage concerns the fact that without feedback, or without further
study opportunities, such a format may not be as effective for retention of
information.
In
summary, SA tests are considered important for learning and recall, since
through this format, students can remember facts, definitions of key-concepts,
and specific content studied. Here is what we recommend to teachers: whenever
possible, provide feedback to make the integration of content more
effective.
Free-recall
Tests that
encourage free recall (FR) can
significantly boost new learning. Such a test format aims to search or
information and content that students have previously had access to in their
mind, without providing them with cues to get the correct answer (Brojde & Wise, 2008). An example is to ask students what
they remember about the topic "solar system".
FR tests
have shown beneficial results across different grade levels and age groups,
with children from two and a half years-old (Cornell et al., 1988), to
youngsters and adults (Tulving, 1967). However,
in the final test of the research conducted by Aslan and Bäuml
(2016), it was identified that younger children (six years-old on average) make
more mistakes when using FR to practice remembering. Older children (eight
years-old on average), on the other hand, benefit more from using FR testing (Aslan &
Bäuml, 2016).
As for the
settings of research on retrieval practice, considering only tests in FR
format, studies conducted in the participants' homes (Cornell et al., 1988), in
the laboratory (e.g., Lipowski et al., 2014), and at school in a classroom
setting (e.g., Jones et al., 2016) were observed.
Research
shows that, compared to rereading the same content, FR influences and enhances
processing in retrieving individual (Tulving, 1967) and specific items
(Lipowski et al., 2014). When comparing the effects of FR (retrieval practice)
with no test (Brojde & Wise, 2008; Roediger et al., 2011b) or with commonly
used study strategies such as copying (rewriting) (Jones et al, 2016; Rowley
& McCrudden, 2020) and rereading (Cornell et al., 1988; Aslan & Bäuml,
2016), it is observed that retrieval practice is one of the most effective
learning strategies that provides longer-lasting learning (Roediger et al.,
2011a). Thus, the advantages of using this type of test are that it enables
memory to be strengthened (Cornell et al., 1988), promotes understanding of the
content presented (Brojde & Wise, 2008), improves spelling accuracy (Jones
et al., 2016), and, as identified by Rohrer et al. (2010), stimulates the
transfer of information to new contexts, benefiting learning in a robust
manner.
In this
sense, FR tests show a direct testing-effect by pushing students to recall
information without cues being provided, thus stimulating desirable difficulty.
This especially benefits the learning of older children. Here is what we
recommend to teachers: provide opportunities for students to freely recall
content (e.g., ask students to write down or comment on what they remember
about the content studied in the previous lesson, without checking any note)
for students starting in elementary school.
Fill-in-the-blank
Fill-in-the-blank
test is a type of test that makes it possible to recall one or certain keywords
(Hinze & Wiley, 2011). For example, in Jaeger et al.'s (2015) study,
students completed sentences after studying a text about the Sun [e.g., The
word Sun is derived from the Latin word ________ (solis)].
This test
format is numerically the least explored in the literature. However, existing
studies have pointed out that this task makes it possible to retrieve items of
previously studied information. According to some studies, fill-in-the-blanks
can aid the retrieval of keywords from an encyclopedic text with 3rd graders
(Jaeger et al., 2015), English language vocabulary learning with 9th graders
(Barenberg et al., 2021), and item information about development, these
presented via PowerPoint to Psychology undergraduates (Vojdanoska et
al., 2010).
In Jaeger
et al.'s (2015) research, students who initially recalled with
fill-in-the-blank [e.g., The surface layer of the Sun is called ________
(photosphere)] - performed better on the final multiple-choice test
after seven days compared to students who reread the complete sentences. The
authors also argue that the practice of remembering through fill-in-the-blanks
can benefit children who perform differently in IQ (intellectual quotient) and
reading ability.
Barenberg
et al. (2021) conducted experiments with German and English word pairs. At
first, they administered the following fill-in-the-blank: a cue (German word)
and the target word (English word). After one week, students who underwent such
a test showed better results relative to those who underwent rereading, either
when they performed final test identical to the initial one, or when they did
it in reverse format (from target language to base language).
Feedback
was present in the research of Barenberg et al. (2021) and Vojdanoska et
al. (2010). The latter's results revealed that when feedback was provided, the
advantages of the test were magnified compared to testing without feedback and
without any activity.
One of the
positives of fill-in-the-blanks is that it is simple to apply in the classroom,
easy to correct, and not very time-consuming (Moreira et al., 2019). However,
there are indications that this format may not show high retrieval practice
effects. For example, in the experiment conducted by de Jonge et al. (2015),
undergraduates studied a text [coherent and noncoherent (isolated sentences)]
and performed a fill-in-the-blanks. As a control condition, rereading the
sentences was used. The authors concluded that the fill-in-the-blanks was more
beneficial for the noncoherent text format than for the coherent one. Thus,
this result may have stemmed from the fact that this type of task does not
require content integration and construction (Karpicke & Aue, 2015). In
other words, this type of task does not enable the student to meaningfully process
and develop more ideas, because its goal is to retrieve/retain one or a few
keywords.
Although
there are few studies investigating the application of this test format, it can
be observed, however, that fill-in-the-blanks presents a simple form of retrieval,
enabling learning to last longer. This format has also proven beneficial at
different levels of education, from elementary school to higher education
(Vojdanoska et al., 2010).
Cued-recall
Cued-recall
(CR) is a test format in which cues are provided to try to recall answers (Lima & Jaeger, 2020). For example, given a fill-in-the-blank
question, the first letter is provided to facilitate recall [e.g., The word
Sun derives from the Latin word s_______ (solis)] (Lima &
Jaeger, 2020). Other examples can be seen in the studies of Aslan and Bäuml
(2016), who used CR tests by presenting two to four initial letters of words
for students to complete noun lists. In turn, Kliegl et al. (2018) presented
blurry (unclear) versions of photos as a cue for students to associate them
with photos they initially studied.
Since it
provides cue to the response, CR facilitates retrieval of previously studied
information and increases the likelihood of retrieving information from memory
(Fazio & Agarwal, 2020). One possible explanation for this advantage is
that the benefits of retrieval practice are more robust when initial recall is
greater than 50% (Rowland, 2014).
CR tests
have been used as a teaching strategy with children as young as
two-years-and-ten-months-old (Fritz et al., 2007). As for settings, research
using cued-recall tests with has been conducted in school, in a classroom
setting (e.g., Ritchie et al., 2013), and individually, in a separate
room with the experimenter (imitating the laboratory setting) (e.g., Fritz et
al., 2007).
The
benefits of CR have already been demonstrated using everything from simple
materials — such as proper names (Fritz et al., 2007), names of sets of
taxonomic categories (i.e., categories that define groups of biological
organisms) and items, photos (with little pixel distortion) (Kliegl et al., 2018), fictional maps (Ritchie et
al., 2013), phrases and word lists with their synonyms (Goossens et al., 2014a)
— to more complex materials — such as sets of concept definition word-pairs
(Lipko-Speeda et al., 2014) and encyclopedic texts (Lima & Jaeger, 2020).
Ritchie et
al. (2013) identified that CR tests — taken based upon fictional maps that
featured the location of some cities as cues and asked students to try to
remember the name and its corresponding location on the maps — driven long-term
learning. On the downside, CR tests, when compared to
multiple-choice tests, resulted in lower recall. Still, both test formats
generate better performance compared to the content rereading condition (Lima & Jaeger,
2020).
The
benefits of CR testing are most robust when immediate feedback is provided
for preschool children (Kliegl et al., 2018). Regarding
undergraduates, CR tests followed by feedback comparing the easy
practice condition (in which the first two letters of the target word are
presented) to the hard practice condition (in which only the first letter of
the target word is presented) showed that mean retrieval performance was
significantly higher in the easy practice condition and in the short term.
However, one week later, performance in the hard practice condition was shown
to be superior (Kliegl et al., 2018). Thus, CR tests allow variation in
difficulty level and therefore adaptation of retrieval practice strategies
according to students' age group and prior knowledge (Fazio & Agarwal, 2020).
In short, the act of presenting cues while
performing retrieval practice can serve as an aid, thus increasing the
likelihood that students will arrive at the answers. Here is what we recommend to
teachers: when providing CR tests, vary the level of difficulty according
to students' prior knowledge, taking care not to make it too easy, but at the
same time pushing students to retrieve information.
Guidelines for educators
The
adaptability and applicability of retrieval practice via different test formats
allow educators and students to use different teaching (teacher-driven) and
study (student-driven) strategies (Roediger et al., 2011b; Agarwal et al.,
2018). There are a variety of question types and test formats that can be used
in real classroom settings (McDaniel et al., 2013) in order to benefit learning
in flexible ways (McDaniel et al., 2013; Agarwal et al., 2018).
To
facilitate the use of retrieval practice, a "retrieval practice
guide" (Agarwal et al., 2018) and numerous
research (e.g., Ekuni & Pompeia, 2020) present
suggestions as well as guidelines for educators to use and encourage the
performance of retrieval practice in the course of the teaching and learning
process. From the research findings presented in this review, we will provide a
summary with the main guidelines (see Image 1).
When using
retrieval practice, students need to be guided that during testing, they should
not consult materials, notes, or even their peers
(Agarwal et al., 2018). They should be honest, searching their minds for
information they have previously accessed (Agarwal et al., 2018). One way to
encourage the whole class to practice retrieval is to be cautious when
directing questions. The teacher might, for example, ask an oral question, give
students some time to think about the answer, and only then draw names of
students to answer aloud.
Image 1
Comparison chart of the studies presented with test formats and
recommendations to educators.
Source: made by the authors.
True-false
and multiple-choice tests can be addressed in real time from quizzes (Agarwal
et al., 2013). One idea is to use colored cards (like little signs) for
students to raise their answer at the teacher's command. It is even better if
the cards have standardized colors, so as to maintain the correspondence
between colors and alternatives (Agarwal et al., 2013; Ekuni & Pompeia,
2020) (e.g., the card with the TRUE alternative can be white, and with the
FALSE alternative, red; with respect to alternatives from A to D, the guidance
is to use different colored cards as well, such as yellow for alternative A,
blue for B, and so on). This way it is easy to visualize the alternatives most
chosen by the students. When asking a question (by voice, by projection on
screen, or by writing on the blackboard), you should give the students time to
think about the answer before asking them to raise their cards. To further
increase the benefit, feedback should be provided (e.g., Agarwal et al., 2018;
Ekuni & Pompeia, 2020).
Strategies
related to short-answer and free-recall tests can be put into practice with
entrance and exit tickets (McDermott et al., 2014; Agarwal et al., 2018). This
strategy can be accomplished using pieces of notebook paper, or bond paper. As
students enter the classroom, the teacher can hand out the pieces of paper and
ask, for example, for students to write down what they remember from the
previous lesson. With exit tickets, it is possible to ask that, before the end
of the lesson, students write down, for example, what they found most
interesting about the topic covered (Agarwal et al., 2018).
Still
considering free-recall tests, it is possible for the teacher to ask the
students to make an oral or written summary of the content previously presented
(Brojde & Wise, 2008). The teacher can also perform dictations with the
goal of having students recall the spelling of words, meanings, and definitions.
Free-recall tests can also be stimulated through interventions during readings
in which the child is asked to point to pictures in the book (Cornell et al.,
1988). Students can also be required to write a list of previously presented
words in a dictation (Jones et al., 2016), or to write down everything they
remember about a previously studied text (Rowley & McCrudden, 2020).
Regarding
fill-in-the-blanks tests, it is possible to use them based on target words
highlighted in a text. At the time of the test, you can present the definition
so that students try to remember and fill in with the target word (Goossens et
al., 2014b). In fill-in-the-blanks tests, one can practice retrieval using
pairs of associated words, matching terms with words, etc. (e.g., suburb and
outskirts - suburb and ________).
In
cued-recall tests, one can present somewhat distorted images as cues and then
ask students to name them (Kliegl et al., 2018). One
can study phrases or lists of associated words and, at test time, provide one
or two letters of the target word for students to try to remember and write the
whole word (Kliegl et al., 2019).
There are,
therefore, numerous possibilities to diversify the choice of test format when
practicing retrieving. It is important to consider that different test formats
benefit learning (McDermott et al., 2014; Agarwal et al., 2018). Furthermore,
offering feedback after practice can help students correct errors and encourage
retention of correct information (Marsh et al., 2012b). It is important to
provide testing not only for the purpose of assessment or grading, but as a
teaching strategy.
Conclusion
In view of an education based
on scientific evidence, the present review points out different ways and test
formats for implementing retrieval practice in a classroom setting. This makes
it possible to contribute significantly to long-lasting and flexible learning.
The results of the studies generally show that each format can contribute
effectively to different content, materials, and grade levels. However, the
present review, by being narrative, has limitations regarding the selection of
papers. Future studies may conduct a systematic review on the theme, by
conducting systematic searches.
From this research, it is
also observed that the age of the students and the level of education seem to
play an important role in the choice and implementation of the test format.
With younger students, providing cues during retrieve may be more effective for
learning. With older students, on the other hand, tests with a higher desirable
difficulty can be provided. Another point concerns feedback. When given,
whatever the test format is, it improves retention and corrects errors.
Therefore, the effects of retrieval practice in an educational setting are
robust. For these and other reasons, retrieval practice is a promising strategy
for teaching and learning for students of different age groups.
Abbott, E. E. (1909). On the analysis of the factor of recall in the
learning process. Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 11(1),
159-177. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093018
Agarwal, P. K., &
Bain, P. M. (2019). Powerful Teaching: unleash the science of learning.
Jossey-Bass.
Agarwal, P. K.,
D’Antonio, L., Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014).
Classroom-based programs of retrieval practice reduce middle schooland high
school students’ test anxiety. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and
Cognition, 3(3), 131-139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.07.002
Agarwal, P. K., Roediger,
H. L., McDaniel, M. A., & McDermott, K. B. (2013). How to use retrieval
practice to improve learning [E-book]. Washington University in St. Louis. https://www.retrievalpractice.org/library
Agarwal, P. K., Roediger,
H. L., McDaniel, M. A., & McDermott, K. B. (2018). Como a Prática de Lembrar pode ser utilizada para
melhorar a aprendizagem [E-book]. Washington University in
St. Louis. https://www.retrievalpractice.org/library
Aslan, A., & Bäuml, K.
H. T. (2016). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older but not in younger
elementary school children. Developmental Science, 19(6),
992–998. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12340
Barenberg, J., & Dutke,
S. (2019). Testing and metacognition: retrieval practise effects on
metacognitive monitoring in learning from text. Memory, 27(3),
269-279. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1506481
Barenberg, J., Berse, T.,
Reimann, L., & Dutke, S. (2021). Testing and transfer: Retrieval practice
effects across test formats in English vocabulary learning in school. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 35(3), 700-710. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3796
Bjork, R. A. (1975).
Retrieval as a Memory Modifier: an interpretation of negative recency and
related phenomena. Em R. L. Solso (Ed.). Information processing and
cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 123-144). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brabec, J. A., Pan, S.
C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2021). True-False Testing on Trial:
Guilty as Charged or Falsely Accused? Educational Psychology Review, 33(2),
667-692. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09546-w
Brojde, C. L., &
Wise, B. W. (2008). An Evaluation of the Testing Effect with Third Grade
Students. Proceedings of the 3-Th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science
Society, 1362-1367.
Butler, A. C. (2018). Multiple-Choice Testing in Education: Are the Best
Practices for Assessment Also Good for Learning? Journal of Applied Research
in Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 323-331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.07.002
Butler, A. C., &
Roediger, H. L. (2007). Testing improves long-term retention in a simulated
classroom setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19(4-5),
514-527. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440701326097
Butler, A. C., &
Roediger, H. L. (2008). Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the
negative effects of multiple-choice testing. Memory and Cognition, 36(3),
604-616. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.3.604
Butler, A. C., Marsh, E.
J., Goode, M. K., & Roediger, H. L. (2006). When additional multiple-choice
lures aid versus hinder later memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(7),
941-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1239
Cantor, A. D., Eslick, A.
N., Marsh, E. J., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2015). Multiple-choice
tests stabilize access to marginal knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 43(2),
193-205. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0462-6
Carpenter, S. K. (2011).
Semantic Information Activated During Retrieval Contributes to Later Retention:
Support for the Mediator Effectiveness Hypothesis of the Testing Effect. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 37(6),
1547-1552. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024140
Carpenter, S. K., Lund, T.
J. S., Coffman, C. R., Armstrong, P. I., Lamm, M. H., & Reason, R. D.
(2016). A Classroom Study on the Relationship Between Student Achievement and
Retrieval-Enhanced Learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28(2),
353-375. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9311-9
Carpenter, S. K.,
Pashler, H., & Cepeda, N. J. (2009). Using tests to enhance 8th grade
students’ retention of U.S. history facts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6),
760-771. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1507
Collins, A. J., & Fauser, C. J. M. B.
(2005). Balancing the strengths of systematic and narrative reviews. Human
Reproduction Update, 11 (2),103-104. https://doi.org/10.1093/humupd/dmh058
Cornell, E. H., Sénéchll,
M., & Broda, L. S. (1988). Recall of Picture Books by 3-Year-Old Children:
Testing and Repetition Effects in Joint Reading Activities. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 80(4), 537-542. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.537
de Jonge, M., Tabbers, H.
K., & Rikers, R. M. J. P. (2015). The Effect of Testing on the Retention of
Coherent and Incoherent Text Material. Educational Psychology Review, 27(2),
305-315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9300-z
Dirkx, K. J. H., Kester,
L., & Kirschner, P. A. (2014). The testing effect for learning principles
and procedures from texts. Journal of Educational Research, 107(5),
357-364. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2013.823370
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K.
A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving
students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions
from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the
Public Interest, Supplement, 14(1), 4-58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266
Ekuni,
R., & Pompeia, S. (2020). Prática De Lembrar: a Quais Fatores Os Educadores
Devem Se Atentar? Psicologia Escolar e Educacional, 24, e220284. https://doi.org/10.1590/2175-35392020220284
Ekuni, R., Souza, B. M. N., Agarwal, P. K., & Pompeia, S. (2020). A
conceptual replication of survey research on study strategies in a diverse,
non- WEIRD student population. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in
Psychology, Advance online publication, 8(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000191
Elias, C. D. S.
R., da Silva, L. A., Martins, M. T. D. S. L., Ramos, N. A. P., de Souza, M. D.
G. G., & Hipólito, R. L. (2012). Quando chega o fim? Uma revisão narrativa
sobre terminalidade do período escolar para alunos deficientes mentais. SMAD, Revista
Electrónica en Salud Mental, Alcohol y Drogas, 8(1), 48-53. https://doi.org/10.11606/issn.1806-6976.v8i1p48-53
Enders, N., Gaschler, R.,
& Kubik, V. (2021). Online Quizzes with Closed Questions in Formal
Assessment: How Elaborate Feedback can Promote Learning. Psychology Learning
and Teaching, 20(1), 91-106. https://doi.org/10.1177/1475725720971205
Endres, T., Kranzdorf,
L., Schneider, V., & Renkl, A. (2020). It matters how to recall – task
differences in retrieval practice. Instructional Science, 48(6),
699-728. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-020-09526-1
Fazio, L. K., &
Agarwal, P. K. (2020). How To Implement Retrieval-Based Learning in Early
Childhood [E-book]. Washington University in St. Louis. https://www.retrievalpractice.org/library
Foss, D. J., &
Pirozzolo, J. W. (2017). Four semesters investigating frequency of testing, the
testing effect, and transfer of training. Journal of Educational Psychology,
109(8), 1067-1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000197
Fritz, C. O., Morris, P.
E., Nolan, D., & Singleton, J. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice: An
effective aid to preschool children’s learning. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 60(7), 991-1004. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600823595
Goossens, N. A. M. C.,
Camp, G., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., & Tabbers, H. K. (2014a). The effect of
retrieval practice in primary school vocabulary learning. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 28(1), 135-142. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2956
Goossens, N. A. M. C.,
Camp, G., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Tabbers, H. K., & Zwaan, R. A. (2014b).
The benefit of retrieval practice over elaborative restudy in primary school
vocabulary learning. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition,
3(3), 177-182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.05.003
Goossens, N. A. M. C.,
Camp, G., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Tabbers, H. K., Bouwmeester, S., &
Zwaan, R. A. (2016). Distributed Practice and Retrieval Practice in Primary
School Vocabulary Learning: A Multi-classroom Study. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 30(5), 700-712. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3245
Greving, S., &
Richter, T. (2018). Examining the testing effect in university teaching:
Retrievability and question format matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 9,
02412. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02412
Hinze, S. R., &
Wiley, J. (2011). Testing the limits of testing effects using completion tests.
Memory, 19(3), 290-304. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.560121
Jaeger, A., Eisenkraemer,
R. E., & Stein, L. M. (2015). Test-enhanced learning in third-grade
children. Educational Psychology, 35(4), 513-521. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.963030
Jones, A. C., Wardlow,
L., Pan, S. C., Zepeda, C., Heyman, G. D., Dunlosky, J., & Rickard, T. C.
(2016). Beyond the Rainbow: Retrieval Practice Leads to Better Spelling than
does Rainbow Writing. Educational Psychology Review, 28(2),
385-400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9330-6
Kang, S. H. K.,
McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Test format and corrective
feedback modify the effect of testing on long-term retention. European
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19(4-5), 528-558. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440601056620
Karpicke, J. D., &
Aue, W. R. (2015). The Testing Effect Is Alive and Well with Complex Materials.
Educational Psychology Review, 27(2), 317-326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9309-3
Karpicke, J. D., Blunt,
J. R., Smith, M. A., & Karpicke, S. S. (2014). Retrieval-based learning:
The need for guided retrieval in elementary school children. Journal of
Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(3), 198-206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.07.008
Karpicke, J. D., Butler,
A. C., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Metacognitive strategies in student
learning: Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own? Memory,
17(4), 471-479. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802647009
Kliegl, O., Abel, M.,
& Bäuml, K. H. T. (2018). A (preliminary) recipe for obtaining a testing
effect in preschool children: Two critical ingredients. Frontiers in
Psychology, 9, 01446. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01446
Kliegl, O., Bjork, R. A.,
& Bäuml, K. H. T. (2019). Feedback at test can reverse the retrieval-effort
effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1863. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01863
Larsen, D. P., Butler, A.
C., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). Test-enhanced learning in medical education. Medical
Education, 42(10), 959-966. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03124.x
Larsen, D. P., Butler, A.
C., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Repeated testing improves long-term retention
relative to repeated study: A randomised controlled trial. Medical
Education, 43(12), 1174-1181. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03518.x
Lima, N. K. de, &
Jaeger, A. (2020). The Effects of Prequestions versus Postquestions on Memory
Retention in Children. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition,
9(4), 555-563. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.08.005
Lipko-Speeda, A.,
Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2014). Does testing with feedback help
grade-school children learn key concepts in science? Journal of Applied
Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(3), 171-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.04.002
Lipowski, S. L., Pyc, M.
A., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2014). Establishing and explaining the
testing effect in free recall for young children. Developmental Psychology,
50(4), 994-1000. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035202
Little, J. L., Bjork, E.
L., Bjork, R. A., & Angello, G. (2012). Multiple-Choice Tests Exonerated,
at Least of Some Charges: Fostering Test-Induced Learning and Avoiding
Test-Induced Forgetting. Psychological Science, 23(11),
1337-1344. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443370
Lyle, K. B., &
Crawford, N. A. (2011). Retrieving Essential Material at the End of Lectures
Improves Performance on Statistics Exams. Teaching of Psychology, 38(2),
94-97. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628311401587
Marsh, E. J., Fazio, L.
K., & Goswick, A. E. (2012b). Memorial consequences of testing school-aged
children. Memory, 20(8), 899-906. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.708757
Marsh, E. J., Lozito, J.
P., Umanath, S., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2012a). Using verification
feedback to correct errors made on a multiple-choice test. Memory, 20(6),
645-653. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.684882
Marsh, E. J., Roediger,
H. L., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2007). The memorial consequences of
multiple-choice testing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2),
194-199. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194051
McDaniel, M. A., Thomas,
R. C., Agarwal, P. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. (2013). Quizzing
in Middle-School Science: Successful Transfer Performance on Classroom Exams. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 27(3), 360-372. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2914
McDermott, K. B. (2021).
Practicing Retrieval Facilitates Learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 72,
609-633. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-051019
McDermott, K. B., Agarwal,
P. K., D’Antonio, L., Roediger, H. L. I., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Both
multiple-choice and short-answer quizzes enhance later exam performance in
middle and high school classes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied,
20(1), 3-21. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000004
Moreira, B. F. T., Pinto,
T. S. da S., Justi, F. R. R., & Jaeger, A. (2019). Retrieval practice
improves learning in children with diverse visual word recognition skills. Memory,
27(10), 1423-1437. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1668017
Ritchie, S. J., Della
Sala, S., & McIntosh, R. D. (2013). Retrieval practice, with or without
mind mapping, boosts fact learning in primary school children. PLoS ONE,
8(11), e78976. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078976
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). Test-Enhanced Learning:
Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science,
17(3), 249-255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x
Roediger, H. L., &
Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). The Power of Testing Memory: Basic Research and
Implications for Educational Practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
1(3), 181-210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x
Roediger, H. L., &
Karpicke, J. D. (2011). Intricacies of Spaced Retrieval: A resolution. Em A. S.
Benjamin (Ed.). Successful Remembering and Successful Forgetting: a
festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork. (pp. 70-115). Taylor and Francis
Group.
Roediger, H. L., &
Marsh, E. J. (2005). The Positive and Negative Consequences of Multiple-Choice
Testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
31(5), 1155-1159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.1155
Roediger,
H. L., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). Inexpensive techniques to improve
education: Applying cognitive psychology to enhance educational practice. Journal
of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(4), 242-248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.09.002
Roediger, H. L., Agarwal,
P. K., McDaniel, M. A., & McDermott, K. B. (2011b). Test-Enhanced Learning
in the Classroom: Long-Term Improvements From Quizzing. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(4), 382-395
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026252
Roediger, H. L., Putnam, A. L., & Smith,
M. A. (2011a). Ten benefits of testing and their applications to educational
practice. Em J. P. Mestre & B. R. Ross. The psychology of learning and
motivation: Cognition in education (pp. 1-36). Elsevier Academic Press.
Rohrer, D., Taylor, K.,
& Sholar, B. (2010). Tests Enhance the Transfer of Learning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 36(1),
233-239. https:/doi.org/10.1037/a0017678
Rother, E. T. (2007). Revisión sistemática X
Revisión narrativa. Acta paulista de enfermagem, 20, v-vi. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0103-21002007000200001
Rowland, C. A. (2014). The
effect of testing versus restudy on retention: a meta-analytic review of the
testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1432-1463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037559
Rowley, T., &
McCrudden, M. T. (2020). Retrieval practice and retention of course content in
a middle school science classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(6),
1510-1515. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3710
Santrock, J. W. (2009). Psicologia
Educacional. McGraw-Hill.
Schaap, L., Verkoeijen,
P., & Schmidt, H. (2014). Effects of different types of true-false
questions on memory awareness and long-term retention. Assessment and
Evaluation in Higher Education, 39(5), 625-640. https://doi.org/10.1080/02602938.2013.860422
Slavin, R. E. (2020). How
evidence-based reform will transform research and practice in education. Educational
Psychologist, 55(1), 21-31. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2019.1611432
Stenlund, T., Sundström,
A., & Jonsson, B. (2016). Effects of repeated testing on short- and
long-term memory performance across different test formats. Educational Psychology,
36(10), 1710-1727. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.953037
Tulving, E. (1967). The
effects of presentation and recall of material in free-recall learning. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6(2), 175-184. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80092-6
Uner, O., Tekin, E.,
Roediger, H. L. (2022). True–false tests enhance retention relative to
rereading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 28(1),
114-129. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000363
Van den Broek, G.,
Takashima, A., Wiklund-Hörnqvist, C., Wirebring, L. K., Segers, E., Verhoeven,
L., & Nyberg, L. (2016). Neurocognitive
mechanisms of the “testing effect:" A review. Trends in Neuroscience
and Education, 5(2), 52-66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2016.05.001
Vojdanoska, M., Cranney,
J., & Newell, B. R. (2010). The testing effect: The role of feedback and
collaboration in a tertiary classroom setting. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
24(8), 1183-1195. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1630
Wiklund-Hörnqvist, C.,
Jonsson, B., & Nyberg, L. (2014). Strengthening concept learning by
repeated testing. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(1),
10-16. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12093
Yang, C., Luo, L.,
Vadillo, M. A., Yu, R., & Shanks, D. R. (2021). Testing (quizzing) boosts
classroom learning: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychological
Bulletin, 147(4), 399-435. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000309
[i] Master Student at Graduate
Program in Education - Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.
[ii] Master in Basic Education
(Graduate Program in Education) at Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná
(2021).
[iii] Master Student at Graduate
Program in Education - Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.
[iv] Adjunct Professor at Graduate
Program in Education at Universidade Estadual do Norte do Paraná.